Frontiers in Veterinary Science 01 frontiersin.org
Owner expectations and surprises
of dog ownership experiences in
the UnitedKingdom
KatharineL.Anderson *
, KatrinaE.Holland
, RachelA.Casey ,
BenCooper and RobertM.Christley
Dogs Trust, London, United Kingdom
Introduction: Although many owners are satisfied by dog ownership, large
numbers of dogs are relinquished annually, with an estimated 130,000 dogs
cared for each year by rescue organisations in the UK. Unrealistic ownership
expectations are a potential factor in the decision to relinquish and therefore
understanding what surprises owners about the realities of ownership and how
this meets their expectations is vital.
Methods: Using a retrospective cross-sectional cohort study design, as part of
Dogs Trust’s National Dog Survey 2021, owners were asked ‘what has surprised
you most about owning a dog?’ and to classify how their experiences had
compared with their expectations on a list of aspects of ownership as either more
than, less than or as expected. Free text responses (n= 2,000) were analysed
using reflexive thematic analysis in NVivo Pro (v.12 QSR) and a quantitative
summary of classified expectations (n=354,224) was conducted in R.
Results: Many aspects of ownership were reported to be as expected, however
a discrepancy between expectation and reality regarding some aspects was
revealed. The cost of vet visits was greater than expected for the majority of
respondents (52%), whilst other factors that often exceeded expectations included
buying/rehoming cost (33%) and amount of patience needed (25%). Damage to
furniture was less than expected for many (50%) as was damage to garden (33%).
From the thematic analysis, four themes were generated that reflected what
surprised owners most about ownership: emotional connectedness of human–
dog relationships; dog’s impact on human health/wellbeing; understanding what
dogs are like; and meeting the demands of ownership.
Conclusion: Overall these results aid our understanding of dog-human
interactions, highlighting the complexity of the dog-owner relationship which
may come with unanticipated costs. Whilst this study’s results are reassuring
given many aspects of ownership were as expected, and surprises were often
positive, some areas had greater impacts than expected, raising opportunities
for intervention, resources or support. The aim would be to manage owners’
expectations prior to acquisition or ensure these are more realistically met,
reducing the likelihood of negative welfare implications for both dog and owner.
KEYWORDS
dog, expectations, human-animal bond, pet ownership, dog acquisition
1 Introduction
Dogs are one of the most popular companion animal species across the world, including
in the UnitedKingdom where dogs are owned by an estimated one-quarter of adults (1).
Although many owners report satisfaction with their dogs and their relationships with them
(2), signicant numbers of dogs face negative welfare situations, such as relinquishment or
OPEN ACCESS
EDITED BY
Mika Simonen,
University of Helsinki, Finland
REVIEWED BY
Jo Hockenhull,
University of Bristol, UnitedKingdom
Otto Segersven,
University of Helsinki, Finland
*CORRESPONDENCE
Katharine L. Anderson
These authors have contributed equally to
this work and share first authorship
RECEIVED 01 November 2023
ACCEPTED 25 January 2024
PUBLISHED 07 February 2024
CITATION
Anderson KL, Holland KE, Casey RA,
Cooper B and Christley RM (2024) Owner
expectations and surprises of dog ownership
experiences in the UnitedKingdom.
Front. Vet. Sci. 11:1331793.
doi: 10.3389/fvets.2024.1331793
COPYRIGHT
© 2024 Anderson, Holland, Casey, Cooper
and Christley. This is an open-access article
distributed under the terms of the Creative
Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The
use, distribution or reproduction in other
forums is permitted, provided the original
author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are
credited and that the original publication in
this journal is cited, in accordance with
accepted academic practice. No use,
distribution or reproduction is permitted
which does not comply with these terms.
TYPE Original Research
PUBLISHED 07 February 2024
DOI 10.3389/fvets.2024.1331793
Anderson et al. 10.3389/fvets.2024.1331793
Frontiers in Veterinary Science 02 frontiersin.org
euthanasia. Each year, in the UK, an estimated 130,000 dogs are cared
for by rescue organisations (3). Factors associated with dog
relinquishment are varied, including dog behaviour, owner ill-health,
relocation or housing issues, lack of time, and nancial costs (4, 5).
Another factor associated with some cases of dog relinquishment is
unrealistic or unmet owner expectations (4, 6). Unrealistic
expectations for ownership were cited as a main reason for adopters
returning dogs to shelters in 7 to 13% of cases (7, 8). Current evidence
indicates a variety of dog- or ownership-related aspects associated
with mismatched expectations. For instance, people adopting dogs
from a UK charity who found the work and eort in looking aer their
dog to bemore than they had expected had 9.9 times higher odds of
giving their dog back to the shelter than people who found the eort
required to beless than expected (9). Similarly, adopters returning
their dog to the shelter within the rst three months of adoption had
signicantly higher expectations for dog health and desirable
behaviour, as well as the development of the human–dog bond,
compared with non-returning owners (10). Excessive nancial costs
associated with dog ownership is another reported reason for
relinquishment that suggests a discrepancy between expectations and
reality (11).
Previous research has explored owner’s pre-acquisition
expectations. In a recent survey of UK puppy purchasers, the
misconception that some “designer crossbreeds” (e.g., Cocker Spaniel
X Poodle, the “Cockapoo”) are “hypoallergenic” and thus pose a
reduced risk to owner’s allergies was found to motivate owner demand
for the purchase of such “designer crossbreeds” (12). is nding
indicates potential risks to both dog and human welfare if owner
expectations regarding a dogs hypoallergenicity are not met. Other
research suggests that owners of doodles (i.e., poodle hybrids, such as
the “Cockapoo”) underestimated the maintenance and grooming
needs of doodle dogs (13). Inadequate grooming can lead to
potentially serious dog welfare consequences (14). In another survey,
conducted in Australia, many prospective dog adopters anticipated
health benets of dog ownership to include increased walking (89%)
and physical tness (52%) (15). Respondents also expected
improvements to mental health, through greater happiness (89%), and
decreased stress (74%), loneliness (61%) and depression (57%). In the
same study, dog training and dog behavioural issues were common
expected challenges of dog ownership, anticipated by 62 and 50% of
respondents, respectively. Expectations of dog ownership are shaped
by experience and knowledge about dog behaviours and ownership
requirements (16). Furthermore, evidence indicates that people with
greater knowledge about animal care, health, behaviour, training and
costs have more realistic expectations about dog ownership (e.g., the
eort required) than people with less knowledge (17).
e relationship between the current perceptions of aspects of dog
ownership experience and owners prior expectations has not yet been
fully explored in a sample of current owners. Whilst there is an
existing body of research about owners’ expectations surrounding dog
ownership, there is a data gap regarding whether such expectations are
perceived to besubsequently met. Furthermore, recent acquisitions
during/since the COVID-19 pandemic has seen a potential increase
to the pet dog population, with negative fallout possible particularly
if dogs were acquired impulsively and/or with unrealistic expectations
of ownership (18). Understanding which aspects of dog ownership
surprise owners, and in what ways, is therefore a critical step in
addressing dog relinquishment and optimising welfare for both
humans and dogs. Using a sample of current UK dog owners, the
objective of this study was therefore to retrospectively explore dog
owners’ current perceptions of their expectations of dog ownership,
reecting on whether certain aspects of ownership were more, less or
equal to what they expected. is study also investigated demographic/
owner factor variables for their association with aspects that surprised
respondents. Furthermore, the qualitative analysis aimed to broaden
the scope of understanding of this topic, by identifying further aspects
of dog ownership that surprised owners and produce richer insights
into owners’ experiences.
2 Methods
Ethical approval for this study was granted by Dogs Trust Ethical
Review Board (reference ERB050).
2.1 Data collection
is study used a retrospective cross-sectional cohort design, with
an online survey used as the data collection tool. e “National Dog
Survey 2021”, developed by Dogs Trust, collected responses from dog
owners in the UK between 10th September to 25th October 2021. Full
survey methodology including tool development, study participants
and data collection has been previously described (19).
2.2 Data analysis
To explore dog owners’ reections on their expectations of dog
ownership a convergent mixed-methods study design inspired the
data analysis (20). Quantitative and qualitative data were collected in
parallel, analysed independently and then interpreted together in a
comparative and contrasting way. Data included within the qualitative
analysis were responses to the free-text survey question “What has
surprised youmost about owning a dog?”, whilst the quantitative
analysis summarises ndings from the question “Aer owning a dog,
please tell us which of the following are less, more or as youexpected?
where respondents were asked to classify 13 functional areas of dog
ownership (Figure1) based on their experiences, as being “less than
expected”, “as expected” or “more than expected”.
2.2.1 Quantitative
Following the data cleaning methodology as described in
Anderson etal. (19), data were imported into R (version 4.1.3) (21)
and the distribution of the data checked. Descriptive statistics were
then collated based on responses to the question “Aer owning a dog,
please tell us which of the following are less, more or as youexpected?,
and variables of interest were compared using the mean number of
surprises (combining both more than and less than expected
responses) cross tabulated to identify dierences between groups.
Variables of interest included age of the dog, due to the potential
dierences in experiences related to the current life stage of their dog,
as well as number of dogs in the household as presence of multiple
dogs may impact the comparison of expectations versus experience.
Due to the previous literature highlighting that younger owners may
bemore likely to experience unrealistic expectations of dog ownership,
Anderson et al. 10.3389/fvets.2024.1331793
Frontiers in Veterinary Science 03 frontiersin.org
owner age category was included within our analysis. Finally,
acquisition factors such as source of acquisition and price paid for dog
were included due to potential dierences in owners’ expectations
when acquiring their dog through dierent sources. Statistical
comparisons were considered inappropriate due to the large sample
size, increasing the likelihood of introduction of false positive results.
2.2.2 Qualitative
From the full dataset where answers were provided to the question
“What has surprised youmost about owning a dog?” (n = 273,899), a
random subsample of 2,000 free-text responses was generated in R
using slice_sample() from all comments that were not blank and
contained at least 3 characters. ese responses were imported into
NVivo Pro (v.12 QSR) and analysed following a reexive thematic
analysis approach (22, 23). e research question that guided the
qualitative analysis was “What aspects of dogs or ownership surprised
owners since their dogs acquisition?” is closely resembled the free-
text question from which the data were obtained. An experiential
orientation to data interpretation was adopted as we aimed to
prioritise owners’ own accounts of their experiences and perspectives.
One author (KH) familiarised herself with the data by reading all
2,000 free-text responses, generating initial codes from the data and
then organising them into meaningful groups from which themes
were constructed. Data were inductively coded, in that coding was
driven by the content of the responses, rather than pre-determined
codes. As coding progressed, some codes were modied. From the
initial coding, categories were identied which were then collated into
themes. e themes linked ideas and concepts within grouped codes
that represented overall patterns of meaning that the researcher
interpreted from the data. Following the same process, another author
(KA) independently coded 1,000 responses from the subsample. is
was done not with the goal of achieving greater reliability or accuracy
between the coders, but rather to deepen our reexive engagement
with the data, for instance by identifying any overlooked areas in our
respective analyses. ree authors (KH, KA, and RMC) met to review
KH and KAs construction of themes and relevant data references, and
collaboratively established the nal themes.
Whilst an inductive approach was employed for coding and theme
development, werecognize that the qualitative researcher always,
unavoidably, brings their pre-existing knowledge to the analysis. As
researchers in the eld of dog welfare research, both coders (and the
wider research team) were familiar with prior research on the topic
being explored. Most authors also had dog ownership experience.
We acknowledge that the teams pre-existing knowledge and
experience may have informed our understanding of respondents
experiences and the aspects they found surprising.
Including the full dataset within the qualitative analysis was
neither feasible, for manual coding, nor necessary, to meet the goal of
this study’s qualitative aspect. In line with research conducted within
a qualitative paradigm, the aim of this study’s qualitative analysis was
to explore some of the range and diversity of experience amongst dog
owners, rather than present a quantied representation of the data.
rough engagement with the subsample outlined above, including
appraisal of the breadth of the study’s aim and the richness of the
individual data items, the researchers determined that this subsample
size had adequate “information power” (24) to meet this study’s aim.
3 Results
A total of 354,046 respondents owning dogs in the UK completed
the survey. Respondents were asked to complete one survey per
household and for their most recently acquired dog. Full demographic
summaries of respondents are available in a previous publication (19).
3.1 Quantitative analysis
Respondents were asked to classify a series of statements based on
their current perception of ownership as either as “expected”, “less
FIGURE1
Percentage of respondents (n =  354,046) stating that their expectations of dog ownership (based on a series of 13 statements) were either less than,
more than, or as expected.
Anderson et al. 10.3389/fvets.2024.1331793
Frontiers in Veterinary Science 04 frontiersin.org
than expected” or “more than expected”, with the overall results of this
study revealing insights in areas of ownership, indicating several
aspects of ownership in which owners reported that their expectations
were commonly under- or over-met (Figure 1). e greatest
discrepancies where expectations were exceeded were the cost of vet
visits (52% of respondents), cost of buying/rehoming a dog (32%) and
patience needed to deal with behaviour (25%). Areas that were oen
less than what was expected included damage to furniture and other
items (50%) and damage to garden (33%).
When considering the number of surprises experienced by
respondents, both “more than expected” and “less than expected”, the
mean number of surprises reported decreased steadily with an
increase in owner age category. Respondents in younger age categories
experienced a greater number of surprises, while older owners more
frequently reported ownership aspects were as expected (Table1).
Younger respondents more frequently reported surprises around:
amount of noise and activity, cost of feeding, and toys/beds, damage
to furniture, social life impacts, patience needed to deal with
behaviour, amount of mess, and time needed for exercise and training.
One exception was the cost of vet visits, which was reported to
be“more than expected” across all age groups; furthermore, the “more
than expected” surprise at cost of vet visits also was more frequently
reported as dog age increased.
A higher number of mean surprises was recorded in those who
owned fewer and younger dogs (Table1). ose with fewer dogs also
more frequently reported that damage to garden and furniture was
“less than expected”. Owners of younger (particularly 0–2 years) dogs
more frequently reported that damage to garden, amount of mess,
time needed for training and patience needed were “more than
expected” compared to owners with older dogs. e mean number of
surprises was also lower for owners who acquired their dogs from
rehoming centres, and higher in those getting dogs from general
selling or pet selling websites. ose who acquired their dogs from
rehoming centres also reported higher frequency of “less than
expected” than other sources.
Finally, those who paid more for their dogs had a higher mean
number of surprises and reported higher numbers of “more than
expected” within their responses (Table1). e amount of patience
needed to deal with their dog’s behaviour was reported as “more than
expected” more frequently by owners that had acquired their dog from
foreign rehoming websites or pet selling websites. Where owners had
acquired their dogs from websites, both pet and general sites, higher
frequencies of “more than expected” were reported by owners for:
more noise and activity, damage to furniture and garden, amount of
mess and mud in the house, and time needed for training compared
to other sources. Finally, owners sourcing their dogs from Kennel
Club websites reported number of vet visits to be more than
expected” more frequently than owners who acquired their dogs from
any other source.
3.2 Qualitative analysis
Of the codes generated from the responses, four distinct but
interlinking themes were constructed: Emotional Connectedness of
Human–Dog Relationships; Dog’s Impact on Human Health or
Wellbeing; Meeting the Demands of Dog-Ownership, and;
Understanding What Dogs are Like. Together, these themes reect
both emotional and practical dimensions of dog ownership and,
overall, illustrate that dogs occupied a more prominent place in their
owners’ lives than they had anticipated. e themes are outlined in
Table 2. Data excerpts contained in Table2 are referenced in the
written account below, to illustrate the themes. In addition to these
themes, some people commented that nothing had surprised them.
ese respondents oen linked their lack of surprise to their previous
ownership experience (e.g., “I have always had dogs so no surprises
really.”), suggesting that owners’ expectations are shaped, in part, by
their dog ownership history.
3.2.1 Emotional connectedness of human-dog
relationships
is theme encapsulates respondents’ surprise regarding the
intimacy of relationships established between themselves and their
dogs. One valued aspect of human-dog relationships, that surprised
some owners, was the company dogs provide (1a). Beyond occupying
a purely functional role in owners’ lives, however, many respondents
alluded to deeper human–dog relationships, commenting that they
enjoyed friendship with their dog (1b), or considered their dog to
be deeply embedded in the family unit (1c). Emphasising the
emotional bond they share with their dogs, some respondents
described the relationship with their dog as though the dog was akin
to a child (1d; 1e). Many owners expressed surprise regarding the
amount or depth of love and/or aection that they felt towards their
dog and/or that they received from their dog (1f; 1 g). e love that
dogs give their owner was oen valued for its “unconditional” quality
(1 h). Owners emphasised a few types of close relationships with their
dogs (e.g., friendship or family) that were oen described as
developing quickly (1i). Many owners described feeling more attached
to their dog than expected (1j), with some commenting that they miss
them when they are not together or that they cannot now imagine life
without their dog (or a dog) (1 k). Whilst some respondents focused
on the attachment they felt to their dog, others noted that this feeling
was reciprocal as they described a strong mutual bond between
themselves and their dog (1 l).
3.2.2 Dogs’ impact on human health or wellbeing
is theme encapsulates participants’ surprise regarding the
impact that their dog – or aspects of dog ownership – was perceived
to have had on the health or wellbeing of themselves or other people,
most typically a family or household member. Most references to
health or wellbeing highlighted positive perceived changes to
psychosocial aspects of health (i.e., mental, emotional, and social).
Many respondents referred to improvements, since dog acquisition,
in areas of mental health, including intrinsic positive feelings of
happiness or enjoyment (2a; 2b). Sometimes these outcomes were
associated with the time owners spent with their dog, including
through specic activities, such as walking (2b), or as a result of the
aection and love dogs give their owner (2c). Here, this theme
connects to the theme Emotional Connectedness of Human-Dog
Relationships. For some owners, dogs were felt to increase feelings of
ease (2c), mitigate loneliness (2d), and provide purpose or motivation
to get up or go outside (2e). Some respondents noted specic mental
health conditions that they felt their dog had helped to alleviate (2c;
2f). Responses suggested that, in some cases, the perceived
improvements to mental health may have been mediated (at least
partially) by the emotional support or comfort that dogs were widely
Anderson et al. 10.3389/fvets.2024.1331793
Frontiers in Veterinary Science 05 frontiersin.org
TABLE1 The mean number of surprises overall, and less than or more than, when classifying 13 statements about aspects of dog ownership as more
than, less than or as expected.
Mean number of surprises
Variable
Number of
respondents
Less than expected More than expected
All surprises (both
more than and less
than expected)
Owner age group (years)
18–24 17,423 2.64 3.27 5.92
25–34 46,273 2.22 3.05 5.27
35–44 52,775 1.78 2.70 4.48
45–54 96,723 1.69 2.45 4.14
55–64 90,272 1.68 2.31 3.99
65–74 43,450 1.84 2.22 4.06
75 or over 8,337 2.18 2.00 4.18
Number of dogs owned
1 256,892 2.09 2.56 4.65
2 96,727 1.44 2.48 3.92
3 19,188 1.26 2.34 3.61
4 5,713 1.24 2.33 3.58
5+ 3,446 1.16 2.11 3.27
Age of dog (grouped by years)
0 36,417 1.71 3.02 4.74
1 42,294 1.82 3.06 4.88
2 33,529 1.79 2.72 4.51
3 33,417 1.85 2.55 4.40
4 31,046 1.88 2.47 4.35
5 29,453 1.88 2.41 4.29
6 27,719 1.89 2.36 4.24
7 26,658 1.92 2.28 4.20
8 25,259 1.92 2.29 4.21
9+ 96,257 1.90 2.26 4.16
Price paid for dog
No cost/gi 49,460 1.95 2.32 4.27
Up to £100 27,398 1.98 2.18 4.15
£100–250 75,169 1.96 2.25 4.21
£251–500 79,672 1.83 2.47 4.30
£501–1,000 87,614 1.76 2.65 4.41
£1,001–2000 45,872 1.77 2.98 4.75
£2001–3,000 14,447 1.83 3.31 5.13
Over £3,000 2,417 1.97 3.30 5.27
Source of acquisition
Local press 6,618 1.76 2.34 4.10
Rehoming website 47,172 1.95 2.14 4.10
Rehoming visit 16,763 2.13 2.15 4.29
Breed group website 10,033 1.66 2.51 4.18
Breeder visit 21,799 1.82 2.47 4.30
Breeder website 13,131 1.82 2.61 4.42
Local community 8,982 1.84 2.46 4.30
(Continued)
Anderson et al. 10.3389/fvets.2024.1331793
Frontiers in Veterinary Science 06 frontiersin.org
reported to provide, particularly during dicult periods in a persons
life (2 g; 2 h). Respondents were also surprised by the increase in social
interactions they had experienced as a dog owner, sometimes forming
social connections with other people through walking their dog (2i).
Dogs were described as acting as a catalyst for conversation with
strangers who they would not otherwise interact with (2j). As well as
interactions and connections with strangers, some participants
commented that their family dynamic had improved since acquiring
their dog, with the dog perceived to have brought family members
together (2 k). In addition to the emphasis that many respondents
placed on impacts on psychosocial health, some respondents also
highlighted benets to physical health, achieved through increased
exercise via walking (2 l).
However, some respondents expressed surprise at how they
perceived dog ownership to have compromised their wellbeing.
Worries about meeting their dog’s needs through optimising their
health and happiness were reported (2 m), with some suggesting that
their concerns were associated with their close relationship with, or
attachment to, their dog (2n). Respondents also related their
attachment to their dog with the emotional distress owners experience
as dogs age and die (2o). is aspect of this theme should thus
beinterpreted in conjunction with the theme Emotional Connectedness
of Human–Dog Relationships. A further threat to owner’s wellbeing
was the emotional strain associated with the oen-reported “hard
work” involved in raising a puppy or owning a dog with challenging
behaviour (2p), which was an aspect of the theme Meeting the
Demands of Dog Ownership.
3.2.3 Meeting the demands of dog ownership
is theme is characterised by respondents’ surprise regarding the
extent of dogs’ needs and how owners meet these. Meeting the
demands of ownership was described by respondents in two primary
ways: (1) through the provision of largely tangible things, for instance
time (i.e., time spent training or exercising) or money, and; (2) how
fullling their dog’s needs was associated with an all-encompassing
caregiving role performed by the owner. Together, these aspects reect
the practical and aective dimensions of meeting the demands of
dog ownership.
e rst aspect of this theme concerned owners’ surprise
regarding the extent of owner involvement or the amount of resources
required to full a dog’s needs. Some respondents emphasised their
surprise regarding the amount of time and attention dog ownership
involved (3a; 3b). For instance, owners noted that the amount of time
required to meet their dog’s training needs was greater than expected,
with some suggesting that they have found training to bean ongoing
process, rather than an activity that can befully completed (3c). For
some respondents, dog ownership had been greater or harder work
than anticipated, oen due to the amount of training or care required
(3d). An emphasis on more hard work was particularly expressed by
owners of puppies (3d) or rescue dogs (3e), with these owners
associating the hard work with inherent challenges they perceive to
accompany these types of dogs. However, several respondents
expressed that the hard work was worth it, given how rewarding they
found dog ownership to be(3f). As well as the demand on owner’s
time and eort, some respondents had not expected the nancial costs
associated with dog ownership to beso high. Veterinary costs were a
commonly reported surprising expense. Some owners perceived
veterinarians to begreedy (3 g), for instance by “upselling” procedures
(3 h) or charging more than owners anticipated for issues they
considered to beroutine or minor (3i). e cost of insurance was also
reported as surprising by a minority of owners, with some placing
emphasis on the rising cost of insurance as dogs age (3j).
In attempting to full their dogs needs, some participants
emphasised the level of commitment (3 k) or responsibility that they
found to berequired of dog owners, or that they felt towards their dog.
A few respondents explicitly associated their sense of responsibility
with their desire to optimise their dog’s happiness (3 l) or meet their
training needs (3 m). Owners’ commitment to their dog’s wellbeing
led them to perceive their lives as being organised around their dogs
needs. One consequence of this was a sense of restriction around
owners’ spontaneity, with some viewing their dog as a tie – for
instance, limiting their ability to travel (3n). A minority of respondents
also noted their surprise that society – particularly hospitality
businesses – is not always welcoming to dogs, adding to the additional
forward planning required when wanting to go out and about with
their dogs (3o).
3.2.4 Understanding what dogs are like
is theme encapsulates elements that comprise how dogs act, or
who they are perceived to beas individuals, primarily in terms of their
temperament, abilities and behaviour. One aspect that commonly
surprised respondents was the amount of personality or character their
Mean number of surprises
Variable
Number of
respondents
Less than expected More than expected
All surprises (both
more than and less
than expected)
Local adverts 2,024 1.91 2.40 4.31
Kennel club website 23,885 1.72 2.60 4.32
Foreign rehoming website 10,559 1.92 2.45 4.37
Family and/or Friends 68,926 1.91 2.50 4.40
General website 31,484 1.83 2.71 4.54
Social media 17,088 1.94 2.63 4.57
Pet website 79,786 1.80 2.86 4.66
TABLE1 (Continued)
Anderson et al. 10.3389/fvets.2024.1331793
Frontiers in Veterinary Science 07 frontiersin.org
TABLE2 Key themes from the thematic analysis relating to surprises associated with dog ownership when asked “What has surprised youmost about owning a dog?”
Themes and
definitions
Sub-themes Example data extracts
1. Emotional
connectedness of human-
dog relationships: owners
were surprised by the
quality and depth of
relationships formed, and
interactions experienced,
between themselves and
their dog
Companionship dog provides “How much Ivalue her company. (1a)
Friendship between human and dog “How Itruly consider him to beone of my best friends.” (1b)
Dogs as part of the family
“How much of a vital role in the family they play. (1c)
How attached youget, they become your children. (1d)
“[Dog name] is the most demanding yet aectionate dog Ive ever owned, hes like a child.” (1e)
Love and aection between human and dog
“I never imagined that Icould love the way Ilove [dog name].” (1f)
How much love and aection they have to show you.” (1 g)
“e love they give is pure and unconditional. (1 h)
Ease or speed of relationship forming “How quickly youfall in love with him and how quickly hebecomes part of the family. (1i)
Attachment between human and dog
I never in a million years realised how attached Iwould get to a dog. I’ve owned cats before but never had the emotional attachment like this.
(1j)
How attached ive [sic] got to him, could not imagine not having him. (1 k)
“e unbreakable bond between me and my dog. (1 l)
2. Dog’s impact on human
health or wellbeing:
owners were surprised by
the eect their dog, or dog
ownership, has had on
their health or wellbeing
Dog improves health or
wellbeing
Dog makes owner feel good
I got more happiness than what Ithought Iwould get, seeing him gain condence and learn is very rewarding. (2a)
“How calm Ifeel and the enjoyment Iget from a long walk. (2b)
How much calmer Iamjust being with them cuddles on the sofa, or just sitting together helps my anxiety. (2c)
“How Ido not feel lonely anymore. (2d)
“[G]ive me a reason to get up even on the toughest days. (2e)
Mental health conditions improved
through dog
“e way it changed the whole family hehelped my daughter immensely as she suers with anxiety and depression. (2f)
Emotional support or comfort dog
provides
“How much [dog name] has comforted me when my parents passed away. (2 g)
“e impact hehas on everyone who spends time with him. Wehave a friend who asked to spend time with [dog name] as it helped him through
a dicult time. (2 h)
Social interactions or connections
through dog
“e friends Ihave made from walking the dogs. (2i)
“[T]alking to strangers because they do not treat youlike your [sic] odd when youhave got a dog with youand youtalk to them. (2j)
How hehas united our family.(2 k)
Physical health has benetted [I]mproving my tness via walking. (2 l)
Dog puts a strain on health
or wellbeing
Worries about their dog
“e emotional drain of worrying about how to help my dog best and what more could Ibedoing to help her. (2 m)
How much Iwould fall in love with him, but also how much then Iworry about him too. (2n)
Heartbreak when they pass away “How devastating it is to lose them its [sic] like losing family.” (2o)
Emotional strain of dog’s behaviour
or training
As our second dog [dog name] has been hard work compared to our previous boy to the point of considering rehoming him. Been soul destroying
at times. (2p)
(Continued)
Anderson et al. 10.3389/fvets.2024.1331793
Frontiers in Veterinary Science 08 frontiersin.org
Themes and
definitions
Sub-themes Example data extracts
3. Meeting the demands of
dog-ownership: owners
were surprised by the
amount of time, work,
eort or money involved
in caring for their dog,
and how meeting their
dog’s needs aects their
everyday life
Dog ownership is time consuming
“e amount of time required to spend with your dog e.g. [sic] Walking, playing, training etc.” (3a)
“Having only had one dog before, a fourteen year old male mixed breed, and gone into owning a puppy with my eyes open, Ihave still been
surprised at how totally full on she is, needing to bewatched every waking minute, in or out of the house! Also, If its within reach, it will bein her
mouth.” (3b)
How dicult puppies are to look aer and how much constant training they need.” (3c)
Dog ownership can behard work
Puppy training is very hard work when the puppy has not got an older dog to learn from.” (3d)
[H]ow challenging it can beto have a rescue dog.” (3e)
How much work it is but how much youget out of it.” (3f)
Dog ownership is expensive
Owners since 1989 […] Vets have gone very greedy.” (3 g)
e behaviour (selling up techniques) of vets, e.g. Routine drip following minor surgery plus pre-op blood tests for puppy tooth extraction and
stitches. (3 h)
“e unexpected costs of vet fees and how everything seems to bea £100 minimum for even just a simple ear infection.” (3i)
Worst thing insurance is a nightmare especially when your [sic] dog is getting old.” (3j)
Commitment and responsibility towards dog
[T]he commitment as wecannot leave him.” (3 k)
How much responsibility Ifeel towards his happiness.” (3 l)
e responsibility to train and teach.” (3 m)
Everyday life organised around dog It is very tying and doing normal things like going out or arranging holidays need much more thought. (3n)
Society is not always dog-friendly [H]ow many places aren’t dog friendly.” (3o)
4. Understanding what
dogs are like: owners were
surprised by what dogs
are like, including aspects
of dog’s temperament,
behaviour or abilities
Dogs’ personality or temperament
“His personality—it’s HIUUGE!” [sic] (4a)
How loyal and loving they are. (4b)
“I have had dogs all my life but never had a Labrador it delights me that how friendly our dog is.” [sic] (4c)
“[H]ow much fun they are. (4d)
“I have had many dogs but this one is so good she has never chewed or eaten anything she is not allowed. (4e)
“e only thing that surprised me is the dierent temperaments of dierent breeds and Ilove how each dog has its own unique personality. (4f)
Dog’s energy or activity levels
is has been the most energetic puppy Ihave ever owned. My last doodle was very calm. (4 g)
“How much she sleeps during the day!!” (4 h)
Dogs’ intelligence
[Dog name] is dierent from all the others iv [sic] had, in that in human terms, Ithink heborders on being a genius, icant [sic] believe how easy
to train hewas.” (4i)
Communication between humans and dogs How astute dogs are. ey know when youare upset or need reassurance and give it to you(all mine have).” (4j)
Dog’s ability to transform
Considering [dog name]’s rescue background when werehomed her at 4 months old how well she has adapted to life with us and how lovely and
trusting she is with people and other dogs.(4 k)
Dog’s behavioural problems
“Hes a sweet dog with a lovely personality, but his barking can upset people and other dogs.(4 l)
“Howling when Ileave him even for short periods. My previous dog had no problem being le alone.(4 m)
“e initial puppy teething stage was a real big shock!” (4n)
“e 6 month old (adolescent) regressive behaviour. Heseems to bepushing boundaries and forgetting all that hes learnt with training.” (4o)
Dogs are dirty or messy
How much bloomin [sic] hair this dog has and leaves everywhere on my carpet. (4p)
How much hepoos and how bad his trumps smell.” (4q)
TABLE2 (Continued)
Anderson et al. 10.3389/fvets.2024.1331793
Frontiers in Veterinary Science 09 frontiersin.org
dog has (4a). Specic personality traits noted were predominantly
positive, including loyalty (4b), lovingness (4b), sociability or
friendliness (4c), and fun (4d). Some respondents were surprised by
how well-behaved their dog is (4e). ere was an emphasis on dogs
individuality as an aspect that surprised people, with some commenting
on dierences between dierent dogs’ temperaments (4c; 4e; 4f). As well
as the individuality of dogs, some respondents reported their surprise
regarding perceived breed-based dierences in temperament (4f). A
further aspect that surprised some owners was their dogs activity or
energy levels. A few people with previous ownership experience
commented on the dierence between energy levels in their current and
formerly owned dogs (4 g). A small minority of respondents were
surprised by the amount of time their dog spends sleeping (4 h).
Some respondents were surprised by how intelligent they
perceived their dog to be, sometimes inferred from their dogs ease of
learning (4i). As well as general intelligence, dogs’ communicative
abilities with people were also noted, with some owners commenting
on their dogs remarkable ability to understand and respond to human
emotions and moods (4j).
A dog’s ability to adapt to their new surroundings surprised some
respondents, particularly those who had adopted their dog (e.g.,
acquired from a rehoming organization). Some were surprised by how
well or quickly their dog had settled in (4 k).
However, some aspects of dog temperament or behaviour were less
positively regarded, as some respondents reported being surprised by
elements of their dog’s behaviour they considered problematic. A range
of specic issues were noted, including barking, which a minority of
participants reported was a problem either for themselves or others
(4 l). Separation-related behaviours (e.g., crying when le home alone)
surprised a few others (4 m). A minority of respondents commented
on behavioural issues associated with puppies, including puppy
teething or biting (4n) and behavioural regression during adolescence
(4o). Dog’s individuality was highlighted in some owners’ accounts of
behavioural issues, whereby individual dogs had been more or less
challenging than other dogs owned by the respondent (4 m).
A minority of participants were surprised by the messy and dirty
aspects of their dog or dog ownership. e amount of hair that dogs
shed was a commonly reported issue, with some respondents
describing dog hair as ubiquitous in the home (4p). Some respondents
reported aversion around their dogs bodily functions, for instance
referring to the amount of poo the dog produces (4q).
4 Discussion
e aim of this study was to explore how the experience of dog
ownership compared with owner expectations in a sample of UK dog
owners. Overall, the results of this study provide initial insights into
certain areas of expectations surrounding dog ownership where
owners may experience discrepancies and surprises related to dog
ownership. e ndings highlight several aspects of dog ownership
that surprised owners in both positive and negative ways, which can
beutilised to guide future research in this area as well as develop
interventions aimed at supporting dog owners to reduce the likelihood
of negative fallout, such as relinquishments, due to these discrepancies.
One key area of surprise for owners within this study was around
meeting the demands of dog ownership, which encompassed both the
extent of dogs’ needs and the capability of owners to meet these. is
was identied through both the qualitative thematic analysis and the
quantitative analysis of graded statements. Within this theme, it was
common for respondents to besurprised by factors related to costs
associated with dog ownership. Common ndings within this study
were unexpected costs relating to the care of the dog, such as feeding
them, providing necessities such as beds and toys, and keeping them
healthy. Previous research has also demonstrated that unexpected or
excessive nancial costs are perceived as a challenge in dog ownership
(15). Within the quantitative data, the greatest surprise reported by
owners was the costs related to veterinary care, with owners reporting
this more frequently as the age of the dog increased, suggesting unmet
expectations can occur some distance in time from acquisition. is
increase in surprise of vet costs in owners of older dogs may either
reect a perceived increase in the costs associated with health
conditions particularly prevalent in older dogs, or a wider perceived
increase in service costs over time. is was also a key sub-theme
within the qualitative analysis where many were surprised at the cost
of care, particularly things they themselves considered minor or
routine. is suggests that some owners may perceive veterinary
surgeries to beovercharging or proteering, when in reality many
veterinary procedures, including routine ones, are cost intensive. With
the UK’s funded public health system, owners may beunlikely to
compare veterinary healthcare costs to that of a human healthcare
setting, raising the question as to how owners benchmark veterinary
costs. Research from the UnitedStates suggests that pet owners and
veterinarians may dier in how they relate to veterinary costs (25).
When discussing the costs of veterinary care in focus groups, pet
owners suggested that animal care should come before prot, while
veterinarians focused on the tangible aspects of their services (e.g.,
time) and felt that their work is undervalued. While some existing
research in other countries has explored public perceptions of the
veterinary profession (26), there is a lack of evidence pertaining to UK
pet owners’ perceptions of the veterinary profession. Further research
into public perception of the veterinary profession and the
veterinarian-client relationship in the UK would be benecial to
understand this further, as well as to better understand cost-related
barriers to obtaining veterinary care and ways to mitigate these. Breed
can also impact on expectations of costs, with owners of brachycephalic
breeds more prone to underestimate the veterinary cost of their dog
due to the high disease burden seen in these breeds (27). Financial
burden may occur where expectations of costs are exceeded. With
nancial reasons oen reported as factors associated with the
relinquishment of pets (2830) and in particular cats and dogs, it is
important that owners are aware of and understand the potential
lifetime nancial costs, particularly routine costs, involved in dog
ownership prior to acquisition.
Successful human-dog relationships oen rely on met expectations,
with ownership oering a symbiotic relationship for both dogs and
their owners. As part of this relationship, it is imperative that owners
invest the necessary time to provide their dogs with what they need, for
both their dog’s welfare and their own, with research highlighting
owners may experience negative wellbeing due to feeling they had not
met their dog’s needs or expectations (31). Within this study, another
area of surprise was the amount of time and eort needed to provide
the necessary care for a dog. Oen owners felt like their lives were
organised around their dogs to full their dog’s needs, and the level of
commitment and responsibility was oen noted as a surprise.
Misconceptions of the amount of time needed for elements such as
exercise and training may exist, particularly to those who may not
conduct sucient research before acquiring their dog or acquire a dog
Anderson et al. 10.3389/fvets.2024.1331793
Frontiers in Veterinary Science 10 frontiersin.org
ill-suited to their lifestyle. A common owner-related reason for
relinquishment of dogs is due to lack of time to spend with the dog, not
being able to provide the time they need (15, 3234), whilst studies also
highlight the increase in responsibility as a challenge of dog ownership
(15, 27, 33). Scarlett etal. (34) found this was most common for owners
of younger (less than 2 years) dogs, with 70% of owners having owned
their dog less than a year, suggesting that in many cases this may bedue
to unrealistic expectations of time requirements, rather than changes
of circumstances. Misconceptions of what specic breeds need has also
been highlighted, with a study highlighting that owners of
brachycephalic breeds were commonly surprised at the level of exercise
and maintenance their dogs required (27).
Training is an important element of dog ownership and, within this
study, the amount dogs needed oen exceeded their owners’ expectations
and there was surprise that this was a continual ongoing process and
need throughout the dog’s entire life. Problematic behaviours are
common reasons for relinquishment, posing signicant concern and
challenges to dog owners (15, 17, 31, 33, 34) and without adequate
training and appropriate behaviour modication, subsequent undesirable
behaviours may develop. Some aspects of dog behaviour were reported
negatively by respondents with many owners being surprised by their
dog’s problematic behaviour. is included issues such as separation-
related behaviour and barking. Previous research has highlighted that
dogs that behaved in unexpected ways resulted in reduced emotional
closeness and attachment to dog – highlighting potential damage to the
human-animal bond if expectations are not met (27). Furthermore,
many owners were surprised by the amount of attention their dog
needed, as well as the amount of patience; this was particularly true for
those owning puppies and rescue dogs. While this unmet expectation
has the potential to jeopardise the human-animal bond, for many the
hard work was however worth it due to the rewards of ownership.
Another key nding of this study was the surprise that dog owners
had around understanding what their dogs are like. When asked to
construct their “ideal” dog, in a study by King etal. (35), owners
reported a number of physical and behavioural traits that were
important to them. is included traits such as friendliness and
obedience, with women preferring traits such as calm and sociable and
men selecting traits such as energetic and faithful. Evidence further
suggests owners acquire dogs to provide a source of companionship,
and therefore likely select a breed reported to oer a suitable personality
and temperament to match their expectations (15, 36, 37). Despite this,
within our research many respondents were surprised by elements of
the way their dogs were with regards to their individual personalities
and temperament, how much character they had, as well as their levels
of activity and energy. It was also common for owners to report
surprises when comparing their current dog to previously owned dogs,
such as dierences in temperament and energy levels. Research has
shown owners may bemore likely to return/relinquish a dog when
comparing them to a previously owned dog’s needs and traits, due to
being less tolerant of what they may consider misbehaviour (15). Dog
needs and behaviour can both vary greatly between and within breeds
(38, 39), and therefore expecting dogs to behave similarly to previously
owned dogs, particularly if the same breed, may result in unrealistic
expectations and ultimately negative outcomes.
e qualitative analysis provided deeper insight into factors not
captured by the quantitative data in this study, such as the emotional
connectedness of the dog-owner relationship, which was oen
reported as a surprise by owners. Existing literature suggests that
companionship is a commonly expected benet of dog ownership
(15), with numerous studies reporting that companionship for the
owner is a primary motivation for dog acquisition (37, 4042).
Companionship for others in the household (including children,
adults or dogs) is another common reason for getting a dog (37).
erefore, this study’s qualitative ndings indicate that the perceived
amount and depth of companionship provided by dogs may beeven
greater than expected. Similarly, whilst previous research has found
that dogs are oen understood as family members (36, 4349) and
that, in Britain, pets have been considered as friends since the end of
the 17th century (50), the closeness of owner-dog relationships
nevertheless surprised many of our respondents.
Consistent with previous literature (47, 51) respondents associated
strong human-dog attachments with the emotional support or
comfort they perceived their dogs as providing them with. Whilst
evidence indicates that prospective owners oen anticipate mental
and physical benets (15, 36), many respondents in this study reported
these as taking them by surprise, suggesting that this is another aspect
of ownership that may be dicult to fully comprehend prior to
acquisition. Despite owners’ surprise regarding the emotional benets
of dogs or dog ownership, this study’s qualitative ndings also
highlighted some respondents’ surprise around the more unpleasant
emotions associated with dog ownership. Feelings of worry and guilt
about whether they were suciently meeting their dogs needs took
some owners by surprise. is perceived negative impact of ownership
on owner wellbeing has been similarly identied in previous research
(51). Perceptions of pets as a source of worry were highlighted in
studies of pet ownership experiences during the COVID-19 pandemic,
with owners worries oen linked to restricted access to veterinary
care, food supply chain issues, or exercise restrictions during this time
(5254). Beyond everyday worries, owners in this study also reected
on their surprise regarding the emotional distress felt when dogs die.
Some respondents’ comments suggest that greater attachment with a
dog may bepositively associated with the severity of grief experienced
or anticipated when dogs die, which echoes ndings from previous
research (51, 5558). Consistent with the contradictory ndings in
existing literature (51) our research suggests both positive and
negative perceived impacts of dog ownership on owner’s wellbeing
exist. Overall impacts of dog ownership (whether positive and/or
negative) on owner wellbeing is likely dependent on the individual
dog, owner and their unique relationship (51).
is study’s ndings showed that many aspects of dog ownership
were reported as surprising. Evidence suggests that around half of dog
owners carry out pre-acquisition research ahead of acquiring their dog
(59). It is therefore perhaps unsurprising that many owners report
elements of ownership as not what they expected. In those that do
conduct pre-acquisition research (and from appropriate sources), this
disparity in their expectations may be due to prospective owners
having diculty in fully comprehending the lived reality prior to
acquisition. In order to address this, interventions could include
physical preparations such as spending time caring for dogs, e.g.,
co-care of a friend’s dog/fostering, on top of the desk-based research
owners are encouraged to conduct before acquiring their dogs could,
in order to prepare owners for the realities of dog ownership. Further
research into expectations around dog ownership is warranted, as well
as assessing interventions aimed at encouraging pre-acquisition
research. Preparing owners ahead of acquisition of a dog may well aid
understanding around the process of acquiring a dog, and reduce the
likelihood of negative fallout such as decreased welfare, damage to the
human-animal bond and relinquishment or euthanasia.
Anderson et al. 10.3389/fvets.2024.1331793
Frontiers in Veterinary Science 11 frontiersin.org
5 Limitations
This study indicates a number of areas that owners may
experience discrepancies in expectations versus experience
regarding dog ownership, however this study’s findings are subject
to several limitations. Firstly, a lack of information regarding
respondents’ expectations before acquisition precludes us from
making direct comparisons between pre- and post-acquisition
expectations and experiences. For example, wedo not know how
cautious or optimistic they were. Our data also did not capture
respondents’ previous dog ownership experience, with previous
dog experience shown to be an influential factor on owner
expectations (16); this limits the conclusions drawn from the data
somewhat. Secondly, our data are cross-sectional, and weexpect
that respondents would have owned their dogs for varying lengths
of time. The length of time the dog had been owned could have
affected respondents’ perceptions and may also introduce recall
bias relying on respondents to recall information from a long time
ago. For instance, previous research has found that dog adopters
expectations for ownership relative to their experience changes
over time: owning a dog was considered easier than they had
expected by an increasing proportion of adopters over time (10).
Finally, the population of dogs within this study could
beconsidered “successful” relationships, given they are still living
with their owner, suggesting that many owners whose experience
violates their expectations may simply manage this. However due
to limited questions in our survey regarding welfare, we are
unable to comment on the impacts of mismanaged expectations
on the welfare or both owner and/or dogs, nor can wepredict
subsequent outcomes from this study, such as relinquishment or
euthanasia. Further research in this area would benefit from
longitudinal study design exploring owners expectations both
pre- and post-acquisition, collecting additional information such
as that listed above within the limitations of this research to allow
for a more thorough understanding of the discrepancy between
expectations and actual lived reality. Furthermore, understanding
how expectation discrepancies and surprises of ownership have
impacted dog owners and outcomes such as relinquishment is
warranted to provide deeper insight in this area.
6 Conclusion
Dog ownership can bevery valuable to humans; therefore, it is
unsurprising that they are most commonly owned companion animal
in many countries, including the UK. Oen attention is drawn towards
the perks of ownership, and the realities and consequences therefore
may not bewidely considered by potential owners. Overall, this study
indicates that owners’ reections on dog ownership are complex and
that this type of human-animal relationship involves forming close
relationships oen at a greater cost (e.g., nancial and time) than
anticipated. is study highlights that particular aspects of dog
ownership, such as the strength of bonds and the extent of the benets
and challenges, may also bedicult to fully comprehend prior to
ownership. Successful relationships, where owners are satised with
their dogs, may rely on preconceived expectations being met and
therefore our ndings suggest a need to instill realistic expectations of
dogs, and dog ownership, in aspirant owners to optimise dog and
human health and welfare.
Data availability statement
e datasets presented in this article are not readily available
because the data are not publicly available due to consent not
collected from participants to share the raw data. Requests to
access the datasets should be directed to research@
dogstrust.org.uk.
Ethics statement
e studies involving humans were approved by Dogs Trust
Ethical Review Board. e studies were conducted in accordance with
the local legislation and institutional requirements. e participants
provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.
Author contributions
KA: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis,
Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Visualization,
Writing – original dra, Writing – review & editing. KH:
Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology,
Writing – original dra, Writing – review & editing. RAC:
Conceptualization, Resources, Supervision, Writing – review &
editing. BC: Data curation, Formal analysis, Writing – review &
editing. RMC: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis,
Investigation, Methodology, Writing – review & editing.
Funding
e author(s) declare that no nancial support was received for
the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. is work
was supported by Dogs Trust and the authors are salaried employees
of Dogs Trust.
Acknowledgments
e authors would like to thank the Dogs Trust Marketing,
Communications and Digital teams for supporting the development
of the research and all dog owners who responded to the survey in this
study. Weare also grateful to Dr. Melissa Upjohn, Dr. Lauren Samet
and Dr. Sara Owczarczak-Garstecka for comments on an earlier dra
of this paper.
Conflict of interest
e authors declare that the research was conducted in the
absence of any commercial or nancial relationships that could
beconstrued as a potential conict of interest.
Anderson et al. 10.3389/fvets.2024.1331793
Frontiers in Veterinary Science 12 frontiersin.org
Publisher’s note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors
and do not necessarily represent those of their aliated
organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the
reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim
that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed
by the publisher.
References
1. PDSA. PDSA Animal Wellbeing (PAW) Report [Internet]. (2021) (Accessed
October 25, 2021). Available at: https://www.pdsa.org.uk/what-we-do/pdsa-animal-
wellbeing-report/paw-report-2021
2. Neidhart L, Boyd R. Companion animal adoption study. J Appl Anim Welf Sci.
(2002) 5:175–92. doi: 10.1207/S15327604JAWS0503_02
3. Clark CC, Gruydd-Jones T, Murray JK. Number of cats and dogs in UK welfare
organisations. Vet Rec. (2012) 170:493–3. doi: 10.1136/vr.100524
4. Lambert K, Coe J, Niel L, Dewey C, Sargeant JM. A systematic review and meta-
analysis of the proportion of dogs surrendered for dog-related and owner-related
reasons. Prev Vet Med. (2015) 118:148–60. doi: 10.1016/j.prevetmed.2014.
11.002
5. Coe JB, Young I, Lambert K, Dysart L, Nogueira Borden L, Rajić A. A scoping
review of published research on the relinquishment of companion animals. J Appl Anim
Welf Sci. (2014) 17:253–73. doi: 10.1080/10888705.2014.899910
6. New JC, Salman MD, King M, Scarlett JM, Kass PH, Hutchison JM. Characteristics
of shelter-relinquished animals and their owners compared with animals and their
owners in U.S. pet-owning households. J Appl Anim Welf Sci. (2000) 3:179–201. doi:
10.1207/S15327604JAWS0303_1
7. Powell L, Reinhard C, Satriale D, Morris M, Serpell J, Watson B. Characterizing
unsuccessful animal adoptions: age and breed predict the likelihood of return, reasons
for return and post-return outcomes. Sci Rep. (2021) 11:8018. doi: 10.1038/
s41598-021-87649-2
8. Hawes SM, Kerrigan JM, Hupe T, Morris KN. Factors informing the return of
adopted dogs and cats to an animal shelter. Animals. (2020) 10:1573. doi: 10.3390/
ani10091573
9. Diesel G, Pfeier DU, Brodbelt D. Factors aecting the success of rehoming dogs
in the UK during 2005. Prev Vet Med. (2008) 84:228–41. doi: 10.1016/j.
prevetmed.2007.12.004
10. Powell L, Lee B, Reinhard CL, Morris M, Satriale D, Serpell J, et al. Returning a
shelter dog: the role of owner expectations and dog behavior. Animals. (2022) 12:1053.
doi: 10.3390/ani12091053
11. Miller DD, Staats SR, Partlo C, Rada K. Factors associated with the decision to
surrender a pet to an animal shelter. J AmVet Ass. (1996) 209:738–42. doi: 10.2460/
javma.1996.209.04.738
12. Burnett E, Brand CL, O’Neill DG, Pegram CL, Belshaw Z, Stevens KB, et al. How
much is that doodle in the window? Exploring motivations and behaviours of UK
owners acquiring designer crossbreed dogs (2019-2020). Canine Med Genet. (2022) 9:8.
doi: 10.1186/s40575-022-00120-x
13. Hladky-Krage B, Homan CL. Expectations versus reality of designer dog
ownership in the UnitedStates. Animals. (2022) 12:3247. doi: 10.3390/ani12233247
14. Watson E, Niestat L. Osseous lesions in the distal extremities of dogs with
strangulating hair mats. Vet Radiol Ultrasound. (2021) 62:37–43. doi: 10.1111/vru.12924
15. Powell L, Chia D, McGreevy P, Podberscek AL, Edwards KM, Neilly B, et al.
Expectations for dog ownership: perceived physical, mental and psychosocial health
consequences among prospective adopters. PLoS One. (2018) 13:e0200276. doi: 10.1371/
journal.pone.0200276
16. Bouma EMC, Vink LM, Dijkstra A. Expectations versus reality: long-term research
on the dog–owner relationship. Animals. (2020) 10:772. doi: 10.3390/ani10050772
17. O’Connor R, Coe JB, Niel L, Jones-Bitton A. Eect of adopters’ lifestyles and
animal-care knowledge on their expectations prior to companion-animal guardianship.
J Appl Anim Welf Sci. (2016) 19:157–70. doi: 10.1080/10888705.2015.
1125295
18. Carroll GA, Torjussen A, Reeve C. Companion animal adoption and
relinquishment during the COVID-19 pandemic: Peri-pandemic pets at greatest risk of
relinquishment. Front Vet Sci. (2022) 9:9. doi: 10.3389/fvets.2022.1017954
19. Anderson KL, Casey RA, Cooper B, Upjohn MM, Christley RM. National dog
survey: describing UK dog and ownership demographics. Animals. (2023) 13:1072. doi:
10.3390/ani13061072
20. Creswell J, Plano-Clark V. Designing and conducting mixed methods research. ird
ed. Los Angeles: SAGE Publications Inc (2017).
21. R Core Team R: A language and environment for statistical computing [internet].
(Accessed September 12, 2022). Available at: https://www.r-project.org
22. Braun V, Clarke V. Using thematic analysis in psychology. Qual Res Psychol. (2006)
3:77–101. doi: 10.1191/1478088706qp063oa
23. Braun V, Clarke V. Reecting on reexive thematic analysis. Qual Res Sport Exerc
Health. (2019) 11:589–97. doi: 10.1080/2159676X.2019.1628806
24. Malterud K, Siersma VD, Guassora AD. Sample size in qualitative interview
studies. Qual Health Res. (2016) 26:1753–60. doi: 10.1177/1049732315617444
25. Coe JB, Adams CL, Bonnett BN. A focus group study of veterinarians’ and pet
owners’ perceptions of the monetary aspects of veterinary care. J AmVet Med Ass. (2007)
231:1510–8. doi: 10.2460/javma.231.10.1510
26. Kedrowicz AA, Royal KD. A comparison of public perceptions of physicians
and veterinarians in the United States. Vet Sci. (2020) 7:50. doi: 10.3390/
vetsci7020050
27. Packer RMA, O’Neill DG, Fletcher F, Farnworth MJ. Great expectations,
inconvenient truths, and the paradoxes of the dog-owner relationship for owners of
brachycephalic dogs. PLoS One. (2019) 14:e0219918. doi: 10.1371/journal.
pone.0219918
28. Dolan E, Scotto J, Slater M, Weiss E. Risk factors for dog relinquishment to a Los
Angeles municipal animal shelter. Animals. (2015) 5:1311–28. doi: 10.3390/ani5040413
29. Eagan BH, Gordon E, Protopopova A. Reasons for guardian-relinquishment of
dogs to shelters: animal and regional predictors in British Columbia, Canada. Front Vet
Sci. (2022) 9:9. doi: 10.3389/fvets.2022.857634
30. Salman MD, New JG Jr, Scarlett JM, Kass PH, Ruch-Gallie R, Hetts S. Human and
animal factors related to relinquishment of dogs and cats in 12 selected animal shelters
in the United States. J Appl Anim Welf Sci. (1998) 1:207–26. doi: 10.1207/
s15327604jaws0103_2
31. Barcelos AM, Kargas N, Maltby J, Hall S, Mills DS. A framework for understanding
how activities associated with dog ownership relate to human well-being. Sci Rep. (2020)
10:11363. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-68446-9
32. Diesel G, Smith H, Pfeier DU. Factors aecting time to adoption of dogs re-
homed by a charity in the UK. Anim Welf. (2007) 16:353–60. doi: 10.1017/
S0962728600027160
33. Jensen JBH, Sandøe P, Nielsen SS. Owner-related reasons matter more than
behavioural problems—a study of why owners relinquished dogs and cats to a Danish
animal shelter from 1996 to 2017. Animals. (2020) 10:1064. doi: 10.3390/ani10061064
34. Scarlett JM, Salman MD, New JG Jr, Kass PH. Reasons for relinquishment of
companion animals in U.S. animal shelters: selected health and personal issues. J Appl
Anim Welf Sci. (1999) 2:41–57. doi: 10.1207/s15327604jaws0201_4
35. King T, Marston LC, Bennett PC. Describing the ideal Australian companion dog.
Appl Anim Behav Sci. (2009) 120:84. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2009.04.011
36. Holland KE, Mead R, Casey RA, Upjohn MM, Christley RM. Why do people want
dogs? A mixed-methods study of motivations for dog acquisition in the UnitedKingdom.
Front Vet Sci. (2022) 9:9. doi: 10.3389/fvets.2022.877950
37. Packer RMA, Brand CL, Belshaw Z, Pegram CL, Stevens KB, O’Neill DG.
Pandemic puppies: characterising motivations and behaviours of UK owners who
purchased puppies during the 2020 COVID-19 pandemic. Animals. (2021) 11:2500. doi:
10.3390/ani11092500
38. Morrill K, Hekman J, Li X, McClure J, Logan B, Goodman L, et al. Ancestry-
inclusive dog genomics challenges popular breed stereotypes. Science. (2022)
376:eabk0639. doi: 10.1126/science.abk0639
39. Hammond A, Rowland T, Mills DS, Pilot M. Comparison of behavioural
tendencies between “dangerous dogs” and other domestic dog breeds – evolutionary
context and practical implications. Evol Appl. (2022) 15:1806–19. doi: 10.1111/eva.
13479
40. Jagoe A, Serpell J. Owner characteristics and interactions and the prevalence of
canine behaviour problems. Appl Anim Behav Sci. (1996) 47:31–42. doi:
10.1016/0168-1591(95)01008-4
41. Kobelt AJ, Hemsworth PH, Barnett JL, Coleman GJ. A survey of dog ownership in
suburban Australia—conditions and behaviour problems. Appl Anim Behav Sci. (2003)
82:137–48. doi: 10.1016/S0168-1591(03)00062-5
42. Endenburg N, Bouw J. Motives for acquiring companion animals. J Econ Psychol.
(1994) 15:191–206. doi: 10.1016/0167-4870(94)90037-X
43. Franklin A. Human-nonhuman animal relationships in Australia: an overview of
results from the rst national survey and follow-up case studies 2000-2004. Soc Anim.
(2007) 15:7–27. doi: 10.1163/156853007X169315
44. Franklin A. ‘Be[a]ware of the dog’: a post-humanist approach to housing. Hous
eory Soc. (2006) 23:137–56. doi: 10.1080/14036090600813760
Anderson et al. 10.3389/fvets.2024.1331793
Frontiers in Veterinary Science 13 frontiersin.org
45. Power E. Furry families: making a human-dog family through home. Soc Cult
Geogr. (2008) 9:535–55. doi: 10.1080/14649360802217790
46. Fox R. Animal behaviours, post-human lives: everyday negotiations of the animal-
human divide in pet-keeping. Soc Cult Geogr. (2006) 7:525–37. doi:
10.1080/14649360600825679
47. Charles N. ‘Animals just love youas youare’: experiencing kinship across the
species barrier. Sociology. (2014) 48:715–30. doi: 10.1177/0038038513515353
48. Charles N, Davies CA. My family and other animals: pets as kin. Sociol Res Online.
(2008) 13:13–26. doi: 10.5153/sro.1798
49. Laurent-Simpson A. Just like family: how companion animals joined the household.
New York: NYU press (2021).
50. omas K. Man and the natural world. London: Penguin (1984).
51. Merkouri A, Graham TM, O’Haire ME, Purewal R, Westgarth C. Dogs and the
good life: a cross-sectional study of the association between the dog–owner relationship
and owner mental wellbeing. Front Psychol. (2022) 13:13. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.903647
52. Ratschen E, Shoesmith E, Shahab L, Silva K, Kale D, Toner P, et al. Human-animal
relationships and interactions during the Covid-19 lockdown phase in the UK:
investigating links with mental health and loneliness. Triberti S, editor. PLoS one. (2020)
15:e0239397. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0239397
53. Applebaum JW, Tomlinson CA, Matijczak A, McDonald SE, Zsembik BA. e
concerns, diculties, and stressors of caring for pets during COVID-19: results from a
large survey of U.S. pet owners. Animals. (2020) 10:1882. doi: 10.3390/ani10101882
54. Holland KE, Owczarczak-Garstecka SC, Anderson KL, Casey RA, Christley RM,
Harris L, et al. “More attention than usual”: a thematic analysis of dog ownership
experiences in the UK during the rst COVID-19 lockdown. Animals. (2021) 11:240.
doi: 10.3390/ani11010240
55. Archer J, Winchester G. Bereavement following death of a pet. Br J Psychol. (1994)
85:259–71. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1994.tb02522.x
56. Field N, Orsini L, Gavish R, Packman W. Role of attachment in response to pet
loss. Death Stud. (2009) 33:334–55. doi: 10.1080/07481180802705783
57. Wrobel TA, Dye AL. Grieving pet death: normative, gender, and attachment issues.
OMEGA J Death Dying. (2003) 47:385–93. doi: 10.2190/QYV5-LLJ1-T043-U0F9
58. Eckerd LM, Barnett JE, Jett-Dias L. Grief following pet and human loss: closeness
is key. Death Stud. (2016) 40:275–82. doi: 10.1080/07481187.2016.1139014
59. Mead R, Holland KE, Casey RA, Upjohn MM, Christley RM. “Do your homework
as your heart takes over when yougo looking”: factors associated with pre-acquisition
information-seeking among prospective UK dog owners. Animals. (2023) 13:1015. doi:
10.3390/ani13061015